In my class today someone made reference to the Kitty Genovese case (it was relevant) and I commented, casually, that I thought that the claim that 30 something people had looked on while Genovese had been discredited. Another student said “oh no, I am revising for a test later today about this” and proceeded to give us the standard account of the case. Here’s Nick Lemann’s New Yorker review of the books that seemingly discredit it.
I sent the students the link, and a different student wrote back that she had thought I was joking in class (they know I do that sometimes) and that as a psychology major she hears about the case in every class she takes. That got me to thinking about the Milgram experiment (which philosophers make much more of than they do of the Genovese case) which, again, seems to me (I say “seems” because I read part of Gina Perry’s book, and have heard her interviewed in depth) also discredited. And made me wonder i) whether anyone has a refutation of Perry’s book but, more, ii) how quickly professors adjust their teaching when findings they have taught as gospel are thoroughly discredited. I was a bit shocked, frankly, that the Genovese case is still being taught as something to be regurgitated in a test, but I am also quite struck by the number of times I have heard philosopher’s call on the Milgram experiment as evidence for some philosophical view, and wondered how long it will take before it is removed from the philosopher’s armoury (and the psychologist’s lectures)
{ 106 comments }
Plarry 09.24.14 at 2:31 am
The Milgram experiments, discredited or not, are still highly relevant in lectures about ethics, ethical design of experiments, and informed consent.
Protagoras 09.24.14 at 2:55 am
There have been numerous replications of the Milgram experiment, and regardless of the issues with Milgram’s original research, some of the results seem to be robust. It doesn’t seem comparable to the Genovese case.
primedprimate 09.24.14 at 3:11 am
Not my field, but I believe that ‘The Bystander Effect’ inspired by the debunked Genovese case (according to the New Yorker link you’ve posted, there were indeed a couple of bystanders but the number was a far cry from thirty!) has also been replicated experimentally by psychologists.
Weaver 09.24.14 at 3:14 am
It’s always struck me that the most compelling evidence in support of the findings of Milgram’s experiment was the behavior of those not generally regarded as the subjects – i.e. the psychologists conducting the experiment. “Why are we disturbingly leading ordinary people to believe they’ve just electrocuted someone to death?” “For SCIENCE!”
Tom Slee 09.24.14 at 3:21 am
… whereas I have always thought that the subjects’ belief (that the experimenters would surely not tell them to do something terrible) turned out to be true (nobody was hurt, even if they believed otherwise for a while), so the lesson might be that obeying a responsible authority is not too bad an idea after all.
Eddie Rosario 09.24.14 at 3:24 am
I remember reading a textbook on persuasion (of all things) where the author took apart AM Rosenthal’s (NY Times) sensationalistic account. Researchers did a study on the Kitty Genovese case and came away with the “bystander effect.”
You can read about here:
http://www.apa.org/gradpsych/2012/09/tall-tales.aspx
Dan 09.24.14 at 3:29 am
There’s nothing at the Gina Perry link to suggest the Milgram experiment has been discredited…?
adam.smith 09.24.14 at 4:19 am
Yes, I’m also a little confused what part of the Milgram experiment is considered “debunked”–what Perry definitely shows is that the most publicized finding–the 65% obedience rate–was only present in a couple of the variations. She also seems to be saying that the protocol wasn’t adhered to(?) or at least that people were badgered significantly more than Milgram let on. But debunk seems quite a strong word for that.
harry b 09.24.14 at 4:45 am
Maybe this is a better link, sorry. And maybe ‘debunking’ is the wrong word, I have to admit I am not sure what it means. Anyway — the point is that there were lots of methodological problems, that the 65% occurs in just one of many variations, that the subjects said they suspected what was going on, and that in over half the 24 variations of the script 60% disobeyed the instructions.
http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/crux/2013/10/02/the-shocking-truth-of-the-notorious-milgram-obedience-experiments/#.VCJKXxaf5YA
Even if both were completely debunked, that wouldn’t mean that the conclusions are untrue, and i know there’s good experimental evidence of bystander effect. What I am curious about is how teaching changes (and how fast) in response to this new knowledge — remember, the student rehearsed the original, clearly false, Genovese story for her test.
Markos Valaris 09.24.14 at 5:44 am
Researchers replicating Milgram’s experiment apparently found large variations between different groups. I can’t vouch for the methodology of the studies, but supposedly 85% of German subjects went all the way, but only 28% of Australians (and only 16% of Aussie women!) did.
That’s reported by Jesse Prinz (2009), “The Normativity Challenge”, Journal of Ethics, citing studies by Mantell, D. and Kilham, W., & L. Mann in the ’70s.
godoggo 09.24.14 at 5:49 am
I do think it’s interesting that Aussie women are less obedient than Aussie men. There must be some way for me to profit from that information. Something to think on. Also I once saw a T.V. movie about Milgram’s experiment staring William Shatner.
godoggo 09.24.14 at 5:50 am
http://www.imdb.com/title/tt0075320/
Markos Valaris 09.24.14 at 5:58 am
I enjoy mentioning these results to my (Aussie) students. But they line up so neatly with national stereotypes that it’s hard not to be a little suspicious…
Susan D. Einbinder, PhD 09.24.14 at 6:07 am
Although Milgram’s study lacked informed consent and voluntary participation, that was not the norm in 1963 when he conducted it. Using deception is still allowed (as long as it is as limited as possible and reviewed/approved by an IRB first). The big “mistake” he made, though, was neglecting to debrief the participants. While I do not know how many, a significant proportion of the sixty-five percent who following the instructions of the authority figure and continued to administer increasing higher shocks to the (confederate) person taking the test developed all kinds of symptoms that manifested over time. But even so, that would not discredit his findings, which strongly suggested that most people defer to an authority figure and do what they are told to do, as long as they can ‘blame’ the bad outcomes on the authority figure rather than themselves.
Although I cannot remember where (or if?) I read this, but my take was that Milgram’s reputation was destroyed because of the consequences of neglecting to debrief his study participants. He was blamed and excoriated for causing severe anxiety disorders and depression among those who thought they’d killed someone. I don’t think he ever contacted any of them to refer them for help or – surprise! – let them know they did not really hurt anyone.
Further, Milgram met tenure standards at Yale, but was turned down because of this consequence of his work: He went to CUNY (where he thrived for 30+ years).
I’ve been on my university’s IRB for 5 years now, and we have approved a few protocols that replicate Milgram’s study with modifications that include, at minimum, informed consent, voluntary participation, and – most importantly – comprehensive debriefing and a referral to an 800 number for mental health help.
I’m pretty sure the Genevese study was discredited before it hit the New Yorker (which was a great article that I assign to my graduate research students in social work), but – apologies – I cannot remember where I read it.
Philip Zimbardo is another poster child for research ethics: He’s at Stanford and designed a study in which he randomly assigned graduate students to be either prisoners or prison guards … and had to suspend the study a few days into it because of the violence and abuse that erupted by the ‘guards’ against the students. But rather than just leave it at that, he actively and comprehensively debriefed and studied it, wrote about it, and continues to reflect on it – you can find his stuff as his website here: http://www.zimbardo.com/resources
Finally, if you can bear it, ask your students how many think that vaccines cause autism. Then inform them that the data in the sole study that generated empirical evidence supporting this causal link was … completely fabricated by a researcher in England. I’ve still got a few graduate students who are parents who are resolute in their insistence that it’s the vaccinations (and this is in a part of LA where lack of compliance is well over the 8 percent minimum vaccination rate to maintain ‘herd’ protection – good think I just have cats).
You can have them visit the federal website that lists egregious violations of research ethics by year that also identified their ‘punishments,’ which are – to me at least – rather mild
Thanks for the opportunity to share research methods stuff!
Neil Levy 09.24.14 at 6:21 am
What Protogaras said. Numerous replications. Don’t throw around the word “discredited” lightly.
dsquared 09.24.14 at 8:36 am
Although I cannot remember where (or if?) I read this, but my take was that Milgram’s reputation was destroyed because of the consequences of neglecting to debrief his study participants. He was blamed and excoriated for causing severe anxiety disorders and depression among those who thought they’d killed someone. I don’t think he ever contacted any of them to refer them for help or – surprise! – let them know they did not really hurt anyone.
I can’t help you whether you read this or not, but it’s definitely not true. It’s basically the opposite of the truth, as a quick Google search for eg “Milgram experiments debriefed” reveals. You know, it’s often a good idea to check rather than relying on hat memory of “I think I read it somewhere” when you’re making very serious claims like this.
Phil 09.24.14 at 8:37 am
Interesting that Perry focuses on the level of variation in Milgram’s replications. There’s been some work recently, by Alex Haslam (U Queensland) and others, which lets Milgram’s reported levels of compliance stand but queries his interpretation, saying he understated the role of pro-social motivations such as the desire to co-operate generally.
But I’m quite suspicious of any claim that Milgram’s been capital-D Debunked, end of story, we don’t need to worry about him any more. I told an older & wiser friend of mine once how I’d read a paper* that called Milgram’s results into question on different grounds again, arguing that it wasn’t a scientific demonstration of obedience to authority so much as a demonstration of obedience to the authority of science, or words to that effect. She said she’d told students about the study many times over the years, and every class’s first reaction was to find a way to dismiss the results – not question or qualify**, but dismiss utterly as totally irrelevant to us here now. Because the thought that those results might be relevant to us here now… no.
*By the literary critic Steven Marcus, fwiw.
**Which to these eyes looks what Gina Perry has done. The book looks really interesting, but I think it’s being oversold.
Phil 09.24.14 at 8:43 am
dsquared – Perry has found Milgram subjects who say they weren’t debriefed. There’s also this paper from 2011, which anyone with institutional access and a better excuse than me can report back on:
Steve Sailer 09.24.14 at 8:46 am
I was hoping that Perry had debunked Milgram, but her critique seemed more discrediting than debunking.
Haftime 09.24.14 at 8:46 am
There was a Milgram experiment done with the learner as a “cute fluffy puppy”, and real shocks were administered (if apparently “without, however, doing the [puppy] any serious harm”).
http://www.holah.co.uk/files/sheridan_king_1972.pdf
Phil 09.24.14 at 8:52 am
Criminology textbooks, I’m happy to say, are a bit more cautious about “Broken Windows” – the totally discredited but still popular theory that environmental disorder leads (by a roundabout route) to higher crime. If you look at the original article, it was only ever a thought experiment – the authors presented it as a common-sensical account of the Kind of Thing that Happens – and it wasn’t so much debunked as prebunked. The authors float the idea that cleaning the place up may reduce crime, but withdraw it and settle for the idea that it will reduce fear of crime, which (they argue) is an objective worth pursuing even if the actual rate of crime increases. The association between environmental disorder and fear of crime, meanwhile, has been studied and has been debunked, multiple times. None of this stops students vectoring it back to me as if it were the results of an empirical study.
J Thomas 09.24.14 at 8:58 am
Yes, I’m also a little confused what part of the Milgram experiment is considered “debunkedâ€â€“what Perry definitely shows is that the most publicized finding–the 65% obedience rate–was only present in a couple of the variations. She also seems to be saying that the protocol wasn’t adhered to(?) or at least that people were badgered significantly more than Milgram let on.
I haven’t ready Perry’s book but I read Milgram’s own popular account of his study.
He tried variations on the procedure to find out what contributed to the results. Like, he found that when it was conducted in the offices of the psychology department at Harvard, he got better compliance than when it was done in a disreputable-looking building in New York. Of course he didn’t get maximum compliance in all versions — he didn’t intend to.
He didn’t hide the amount he badgered the participants. He had a list of phrases he was allowed to say, and he made films of himself doing it and saying them.
Perry’s book gets more publicity if it looks like she’s saying something new, but I can’t imagine what would be there to discredit the results. Yes, when people objected he told them they had to, that they had no choice. He said “The experiment requires that you continue.” So? Didn’t the Nazis tell people they had to, and that the Fatherland required that they continue?
eur 09.24.14 at 10:15 am
Since there are several cases like this that have become entrenched as tools to vividly exemplify a claim in academic education and research it would be very useful to have a website dedicated to them. I’m thinking of something like snopes.com but more rigorous and targeted at helping teachers with what the case, given the best available evidence now, can and cannot be used to exemplify. If a vivid study that purported to prove a mechanism X has been discredited, but the mechanism X has been verified elsewhere, then the site could suggest other ways to vividly exemplify the mechanism.
Phil 09.24.14 at 10:16 am
Actually I don’t think the Nazis did. I think they strongly encouraged, and valorised, active participation and very strongly discouraged active resistance, but I don’t think they devoted too much attention to people who did a Bartleby & preferred not to join in – other than by interpreting passivity as active resistance, when it suited them to. Nazism was all about the positive buy-in. Not so much “you must continue”; more “you’re not continuing? very well, it’s your choice” interspersed with “you’re not continuing? but only a spy or a traitor would not continue!”.
novakant 09.24.14 at 10:49 am
“I … comme Icare” – great film:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=G9_gnsEdUCg
novakant 09.24.14 at 10:50 am
the text link is the first part
godoggo 09.24.14 at 11:23 am
All that stuff about the harm to Milgram’s career reminded me of this paragraph (and a similar ones) from a Salon article the other day:
“From 1960 until 1971, Dr. Eugene Saenger, a radiologist at the University of Cincinnati, led an experiment exposing 88 cancer patients, poor and mostly black, to whole body radiation, even though this sort of treatment had already been pretty well discredited for the types of cancer these patients had. They were not asked to sign consent forms, nor were they told the Pentagon funded the study. They were simply told they would be getting a treatment that might help them. Patients were exposed, in the period of one hour, to the equivalent of about 20,000 x-rays worth of radiation. Nausea, vomiting, severe stomach pain, loss of appetite, and mental confusion were the results. A report in 1972 indicated that as many as a quarter of the patients died of radiation poisoning. Dr. Saenger recently received a gold medal for “career achievements†from the Radiological Society of North America.”
http://www.salon.com/2014/09/04/10_of_the_most_evil_medical_experiments_in_history_partner/
Oh, I just thought of a snappy punchline! Too bad Milgram didn’t have the foresight to arrange a contract with the Pentagon like that guy! Or something.
PJW 09.24.14 at 11:29 am
The so-called Monster Study in Iowa from 1939 on stuttering might be of interest:
http://www-psych.stanford.edu/~bigopp/stutter2.html
PJW 09.24.14 at 11:32 am
Sorry for the redundancy as the Monster Study is listed in godoggo’s Salon article.
Thornton Hall 09.24.14 at 11:55 am
One way to avoid getting it wrong: just ignore the subject entirely. Doctors get their nutrition science from the news.
harry b 09.24.14 at 12:54 pm
In a meeting recently, discussing how to get the press to report better on educational research, one very eminent scholar said “well, at least nobody knows anything about educational research, so we’re not in the position of the people doing research on diet — where the press reports that eggs are bad for you, and then they’re good for you, and then they’re bad for you, and you can’t have coffee with bacon but then you can have coffee with bacon and in the end the public are sensible enough not to believe anything they read”. Except… people still seem to believe what they read.
harry b 09.24.14 at 1:08 pm
NOBODY has talked about the main question I asked — how quickly does teaching change in response to changes in the science. Anyone know about teaching in nutrition science.
AcademicLurker 09.24.14 at 1:13 pm
32: Nutrition journalism is totally worthless, hopefully the quality of the teaching in nutrition science programs is somewhat better.
Chris Adams 09.24.14 at 1:17 pm
Ignoring the question of whether the Milgram experiments have actually been discredited, there’s a big difference in the way the incorrect Genovese narrative has been politically useful for some powerful groups. That’s given it decades of consistent reinforcement to the point where anyone who isn’t consciously making an effort to confront it will be influenced by the fictional version.
bianca steele 09.24.14 at 1:18 pm
Phil @ 18
Re. the chronology in your source: My husband was a psychology minor thirty years ago, and would be quite surprised to hear that at that time there was any need for Milgram’s “rehabilitation.”
bianca steele 09.24.14 at 1:53 pm
The link @6, incidentally, is a bit shocking in the researchers’ insistence that though people, interviewed later about what they did at the time in the Genovese case, said they called the police, we can be perfectly sure that they did not. I wonder what the response would be to journalists’ actually having found records in the archive of those calls? Either way, it seems hardly “scientific” to make those kinds of pronouncements about specific, in some cases named, people
AcademicLurker 09.24.14 at 2:20 pm
Signing on with 34. There was a powerful racist and anti-urban political sentiment that the Genovese narrative reenforced, which was a big factor in how far and fast it spread and how persistent it’s been over the years.
I don’t think the Milgram experiment is quite analogous in that respect.
Dave Maier 09.24.14 at 2:38 pm
that 30 something people had looked on while Genovese had been discredited
And they did nothing? Did none of them care about her intellectual reputation?
Phil 09.24.14 at 4:16 pm
bianca – I haven’t actually read the paper, so I’m not taking any position on its veracity. But the abstract I quoted doesn’t say anything either way about Milgram’s standing thirty years ago.
Alex 09.24.14 at 4:33 pm
The most interesting thing I learned from that New Yorker article was that Kitty Genovese and one of the witnesses who delayed calling the police were both gay. We never learned that in Psych 101. The papers erased her identity and relationship in the effort to portray her as the innocent victim of a pervert, and the gay man who didn’t call the police immediately was probably justified in his fear of them. That’s not about diluted responsibility; that’s about what happens in communities who are more afraid of the police than of criminals, because the police habitually beat, rape, arrest, and kill members of the community with impunity. Something law enforcement still needs to learn today.
Bruce Wilder 09.24.14 at 4:53 pm
We’re always revising our stories, and generational re-telling of stories is akin to a game of telephone or rumour.
The re-telling itself ought to be subject to narrative analysis and examination. The re-telling — particularly the age-old academic tradition of applying allegedly critical method to the de-bunking of the myth — is surely as motivated as the original impulse to make the legend fact, and print the legend.
The Milgram experiments, or the Kitty Genovese case, or any number of other legendary incidents, are important because they challenge us emotionally and philosophically, and precisely because they challenge us in our embrace of certain primitives, we divide ourselves in ambivalent reactions that make us want the story to accommodate our prejudices, and, at the same time, we want the story to conform to the dramatic conventions that make it memorable and meaningful to begin with.
We recently had a thread that turned on recalling the Nazi persecution and killing of Jews and other civilians during World War II, and the emotional impact that the confirmation of that activity — particularly the dramatic photographs of the concentration camps taken when the camps were overrun or discovered by Allied troops — had on the world, on Allied conceptions of their war aims, etc.
It was pointed out in that thread that the infamous photographs were of prisoners in “concentration camps”, which scholars distinguish from the “extermination camps”, where the industrialized mass killing took place (and which had been demolished before the end of the war.) The photographs — of emaciated prisoners and corpses piled up like cordwood — have a dramatic impact and “truth” to them. What is the point of introducing that distinction as a contradiction? “These aren’t the death camps — this isn’t the holocaust.” Is it to establish truth? Or, to escape it?
The OP was about a film made about the concentration camps at the end of WWII with the aim of confronting the German population with the gross inhumanity of their conduct as a body politic, forcing them to “see” the “truth”, and there was some back and forth in the thread about whether the Germans could be said to be “aware” of what was going on in the concentration camps before the end of the war, or of the extermination policies.
Denial is a basic human capacity — maybe so basic to politics that it could be termed a political instinct. Every story, whose dramatic elements make it memorable, emotionally resonant and morally meaningful necessarily packs a lot of detail away out of sight, and it can be unpacked in re-telling, the additional details — documented or merely imagined — teased out to suggest different, revised meaning to the audience, even as the re-telling, itself, builds a myth or erodes the emotional impact into ritual and cliché.
Narrators have purposes and motivations: they want to instruct their audience, I suppose, but they also want an audience. The spirit of “when the legend becomes fact, print the legend” is the spirit that sells newspapers, but it reflects a demand that shapes the story as much as the motivations of the narrator.
The old joke about the classical scholar, who, after a decade of laboring in obscurity, mounted the podium at a conference to great anticipation, to announce that he had established beyond doubt that the Iliad had not been composed by the legendary Homer, but by another ionic Greek poet of the same name, suggests that revisionism is built into the enterprise, that the pursuit of trivial detail becomes an end in itself.
But, revisionism is also built into the ever-increasing distance we have to the past, and the ever-increasing volume of the past in relation to the present. Our stories multiply and we have to squeeze them down, crowd them out. The Milgram experiment had its initial impact in the shadow of the authoritarian politics that produced WWII and its horrors. The Authoritarian Personality, Adorno et alia, was still social science at its most ambitious. That generation had seen Armageddon; they could afford their realism.
Now, we are in the shadow of Dr Pangloss’ neoliberal nightmare, a brittle story of wilful optimism; the epitome of social science is an economics that is clearly wrong about everything and lies about the rest. Our weary cynicism weighs heavily, as we pretend that the costs of coping will be small, as we labor to avoid the Armageddon looming ahead; we cannot afford realism. (Did I just switch comment threads? Ooops!)
TM 09.24.14 at 5:39 pm
Totally different area, but one of the most influential and most widely cited scientific papers ever – Garrett Hardin’s “Tragedy of the Commons” – is based on a historically widely inaccurate portrayal of the management of common resources in traditional societies. The claim of the author (who had no expertise whatsoever in the field) that “the inherent logic of the commons remorselessly generates tragedy” has been heavily criticized by historically more knowledgeable authors, e.g. economist Partha Dasgupta wrote: “it is difficult to find a passage of comparable length and fame that contains so many errors as the one quoted†. There is little evidence for systematic overgrazing in traditional commons and plenty of evidence for overuse of privately owned resources, yet the “Tragedy of the Commons” is routinely cited (usually by people who haven’t read the essay) as a strong argument for the privatization of common resources. Amazingly, most economists and conservationists teaching the “Tragedy of the Commons”, including text book authors, are totally unaware of the criticisms and tend to take the concept as gospel.
http://www.slideshare.net/amenning/tragedy-commons
http://www.monbiot.com/1994/01/01/the-tragedy-of-enclosure/
TM 09.24.14 at 5:49 pm
21: “Broken Windows” is another good example along the same lines. I also second 23:
“Since there are several cases like this that have become entrenched as tools to vividly exemplify a claim in academic education and research it would be very useful to have a website dedicated to them.”
Generally anybody teaching philosophy or sociology of science might want to spend some time discussing these cases.
TM 09.24.14 at 5:57 pm
31: “well, at least nobody knows anything about educational research,”
Not true anymore. Education research is becoming popular, perhaps not yet as popular as nutrition fads… See http://www.washingtonpost.com/blogs/wonkblog/wp/2014/06/10/a-california-judge-just-ruled-that-teacher-tenure-is-bad-for-students/. Note that this research is already discredited (just look at the charts with superstretched axes; see http://nepc.colorado.edu/blog/moshe-adler-chetty for more) but who cares.
David in NY 09.24.14 at 6:09 pm
If the Genovese case is still being taught as Rosenthal’s version, a full 38 people witnessing a gruesome crime and doing absolutely nothing because they didn’t want to get “involved” (the only version I knew, as passed on by Phil Ochs’s A Small Circle of Friends, until a few years ago), it does raise the question not much addressed here. Why? There has been a fair amount of press about the actual situation, as described persuasively in trial testimony and other evidence, that it makes the Genovese case hardly emblematic of anything. Some people did stuff to help (it is indisputable that Mosley was scared away by one, someone called an ambulance, one went to comfort her), some, of weaker disposition, did not. It seems unsupported that there were 38 witnesses.
In addition, the subsequent social research on when people will help others doesn’t suggest any general indifference among members of the public to help those in distress, but rather calculations made based on the apparent need for intervention and the dangers involved in it.
You’d think that a bunch of PhD’s could at least get the facts right. I think that the Genovese story was fundamentally an anti-urban tale — how impersonal the big city is. My guess is that the problem here is that that is the default view of most Americans. This stuff happens, but it wouldn’t in their little town. And professors are not immune to such simple-minded default schemes.
JanieM 09.24.14 at 6:45 pm
This stuff happens, but it wouldn’t in their little town.
You can pretty much count on someone being quoted as saying this exact thing whenever a murder happens in a small town. “This doesn’t happen here.”
Aaron Lercher 09.24.14 at 8:37 pm
Milgram did not get the same result of 65% obedience in every experimental run. Milgram varied the experimental conditions, and then observed a varying rate of obedience. This seems to show that Milgram identified something like a mechanism of obedience. (The closer the subject was to the “victim” the lower the obedience rate.) The variation of the obedience rate is evidence in favor of Milgram’s account of obedience. It does not debunk Milgram.
Milgram’s deceptive manipulation of experimental subjects is hard to replicate today. If Milgram mistreated experimental subjects, this does not refute his results.
harry b 09.24.14 at 9:31 pm
Obviously these things don’t happen in little towns, hardly anyone lives in them. They’re little.
bianca steele 09.24.14 at 9:34 pm
Phil,
I’m not sure what you’re saying. Does “over the past 20 years, there has been a gradual rehabilitation of Milgram’s work and reputation,” have a different interpretation than that around 1991, Milgram’s reputation was around its nadir, and probably had been low for a long time? Thirty years ago is 1984, only seven years before that. Maybe there’s documentation in the paper (which neither of us have access to) for the history. Or maybe it’s based on the consensus at the writer’s own university. Possibly “reputation” should be amended to “reputation in the UK” or wherever. But I see no way of sanely contending that the abstract doesn’t make claims about what qualified people believed in the years just before 1991 or so.
Incidentally, my memory of being taught/having read about this is that the argument relied on there being absolutely no browbeating of the subjects. “The experiment is flawed but it’s important to teach this lesson, even if anybody who reads about the experiment can see it doesn’t teach a clear lesson,” is odd, and this seems like an expensive way of going about teaching it.
LFC 09.24.14 at 10:36 pm
David in NY@45
My guess is that the problem here is that that is the default view of most Americans. This stuff happens, but it wouldn’t in their little town.
But the majority of Americans no longer live in small towns, rather in cities and/or their suburbs and exurbs. The impersonality-of-the-big-city theme might have been a default view several decades ago, but I’m not sure it still is.
Phil 09.24.14 at 11:11 pm
bianca – I think the main thing I’m saying is that there’s no point arguing about the abstract of a paper neither of us has read. The paper may be completely wrong about Milgram’s reputation, or it may not. I just thought it was interesting that somebody writing in a journal seems to have cast doubt on Milgram’s debriefing practices.
Bruce:
It was pointed out in that thread that the infamous photographs were of prisoners in “concentration campsâ€, which scholars distinguish from the “extermination campsâ€, where the industrialized mass killing took place (and which had been demolished before the end of the war.) The photographs — of emaciated prisoners and corpses piled up like cordwood — have a dramatic impact and “truth†to them. What is the point of introducing that distinction as a contradiction? “These aren’t the death camps — this isn’t the holocaust.†Is it to establish truth? Or, to escape it?
I grew up with the vague impression that the Nazis had places called concentration camps in which they killed Jews, and that Dachau and Belsen were two examples. I’m sure lots of other people grew up with similar confused ideas. It’s a confusion that plays into the hands of Holocaust revisionists – who make great play of the fact that many of the best-known concentration camps weren’t death camps, using it to imply that the rest weren’t either. So yes, it’s very much to establish truth.
Saying that those pictures from the liberation of Belsen and other western camps – emaciated prisoners and corpses piled up like cordwood – weren’t part of the Holocaust doesn’t in any way lessen the reality of how bad they were. On the contrary, it says that the Nazis were guilty of these appalling crimes against humanity and of even worse things.
L.D. Burnett 09.24.14 at 11:13 pm
Here’s a very recent article on psychologists’ reassessment of the Milgram experiments — this is an article from the Independent, previewing the forthcoming documentary Shock Room:
http://www.independent.co.uk/news/science/famous-milgram-electric-shocks-experiment-drew-wrong-conclusions-about-evil-say-psychologists-9712600.html
Here’s the key takeaway: Per an expert quoted in the article, “It appears from this feedback that the main reason participants weren’t distressed is that they did not think they had done anything wrong. This was largely due to Milgram’s ability to convince them that they had made an important contribution to science.”
Phil 09.24.14 at 11:16 pm
NOBODY has talked about the main question I asked — how quickly does teaching change in response to changes in the science.
Not quite true. In the case of one famous and influential argument in criminology (as I said above) it probably didn’t take more than a decade for textbooks to start pointing students to the debunkings – the better class of textbook, at least. There’s still a bit of “teach the controversy” going on, but that’s probably unavoidable in a discipline with one foot in public policy – “Broken Windows” has been insanely influential out there in the real world.
Phil 09.24.14 at 11:18 pm
LDB – yes, that’s the Haslam et al research I referred to above. Nice to see it getting some traction.
L.D. Burnett 09.24.14 at 11:32 pm
Phil, I didn’t realize that was the same study, but I see you’re right — the quote I posted above is from Haslam.
What I find most interesting about Haslam’s research is what it suggests — or serves to confirm — regarding the esteem in which people held science/the scientific, the belief that the advancement of scientific knowledge was a social good that trumped other concerns, etc.
Abbe Faria 09.24.14 at 11:39 pm
“NOBODY has talked about the main question I asked — how quickly does teaching change in response to changes in the science.”
I think there’s a real disjunct between teaching and research in social psychology. Current research is a mess, the field’s in crisis, they keep on ‘discovering’ minor effects – stereotype threat, social priming, etc – which they can’t replicate.
On the other hand, they do have an amazing collection of research bequeathed to them from the post-war high impact experimental drama school of social psychology (the cool stuff; the prison, bystander, conformity, cognitive disonance experiments, etc). So this is what they teach.
Most of this would be unpublishable in journals today and doesn’t speak to the fields current concerns. So you do get regular atempts to trash what was very morally relevant and humanitarian research scheme (see above). But marketing purposes that’s what they have to lead Soc Psych 101 with, even if that style of research is currently held in contempt, more recent work doesn’t sell as well.
ChrisB 09.24.14 at 11:55 pm
“The old joke about the classical scholar, who, after a decade of laboring in obscurity, mounted the podium at a conference to great anticipation, to announce that he had established beyond doubt that the Iliad had not been composed by the legendary Homer, but by another ionic Greek poet of the same name”
I was about to confidently correct the writer by pointing out that this was a Woody Allen joke about Shakespeare, but a fortuitous check turned up proof that the quip dates from 1860, has been said about a number of writers, and was used by Twain, Chesterton, and Huxley, among others. A cautionary tale.
mdc 09.25.14 at 12:08 am
I still hear people (including npr) refer to the mass panic caused by the War of the Worlds broadcast.
burritoboy 09.25.14 at 12:11 am
“But the majority of Americans no longer live in small towns, rather in cities and/or their suburbs and exurbs. The impersonality-of-the-big-city theme might have been a default view several decades ago, but I’m not sure it still is.”
Perceptions like that have sometimes tangential relationships to any reality. The majority of Americans, for example, live in suburbs – suburbs which they pretend to themselves are small towns. This is not a trivial perception for many Americans – if you suggest that what they prefer to perceive as their small independent town is really a suburb dependent on the nearby major city, do not be surprised if they vociferously object. This has a huge impact on American politics.
Keep your eyes peeled and observe how American suburbs are designed.
For another example, look at the hold the ideal of “the country” has in the English imagination. It’s been a very long time indeed since the majority of English citizens have really lived in truly rural areas (130-150 or more years at this point), but the “country” imagery still has an extremely powerful hold over huge swathes of English urban planning, architecture, fashions, education, policies around crime and so on.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 3:00 am
LFC @ 50. The “little town” was metaphorical. I think the anti-urban feeling is still very real — people who don’t live in the city, but live even a little outside (and the city can be one of 100,000 or 1 million), say they would never want to live there. And indeed, people in the city often say pretty much the same thing. It’s the American way. And I think the Genovese story fits right into it.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 3:04 am
Well, I see burritoboy had covered much the same territory I did, in #59 which I had not noticed when I wrote #60. I pretty much agree with him.
Shatterface 09.25.14 at 3:12 am
In linguistics, the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis is – at least in its strong form – largely discredited but it is generally taken for granted by science fiction writers.
Shatterface 09.25.14 at 3:14 am
Come to think of it, they still teach psychoanalysis.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 3:19 am
harry b @ 50. “Obviously these things don’t happen in little towns, hardly anyone lives in them. They’re little.”
The point is that the Genovese story didn’t happen in a city either. But people, including people who ought to know better, are willing to believe it did, primarily because it was set in a place they view as alien to them, where people would uniformly act in a callous manner. And it is this kind of widespread anti-urban view that protects this story from the debunking that you suggest ought to be so easy, and indeed should be, especially by people (say teachers) who ought to know better.
That’s my explanation for the slowness to overcome that one. Be happy to hear an alternative explanation.
Meredith 09.25.14 at 4:52 am
To expand this discussion beyond science and social science research, which focuses more or less on the present (whether eternal or ephemeral present)…. How about claims routinely made about past societies studied in the context of the humanities? Classicists (for instance) constantly hear or read mischaracterizations of ancient Mediterranean societies even by learned colleagues in other fields (and don’t get me started on the U.S. English AP tests, which promote readings of Aristotle’s Poetics from several centuries ago). Or, take the ongoing assumption that there are no Indians left in the eastern U.S (not true, of course). The list is obscenely long. Sometimes misinformation/misunderstanding persists due to simple, honest ignorance — none of us can keep up with all the expertise in all fields — but often it persists because of the ideological work it does. I had occasion once to challenge Edward Said about his treatment of Aeschylus’ Persians in Orientalism (a book I highly respect, on the whole, and Said was a man and a scholar whom I highly respect still). He was apologetic about using that play in a rhetorical strategy of “starting with the Greeks” (an old way of doing things, of course, and a problematic one — as he recognized). “The Greeks” as origin does a lot of ideological work quickly, efficiently. But our understanding of Greeks and the past, and thus of our present and potential futures, thereby also suffers.
Peter Dorman 09.25.14 at 5:35 am
TM, 42: Amazingly, most economists and conservationists teaching the “Tragedy of the Commonsâ€, including text book authors, are totally unaware of the criticisms and tend to take the concept as gospel.
I know of one introductory economics textbook that follows up a summary of Hardin with a summary of Ostrom.
Shatterface, 62: In linguistics, the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis is – at least in its strong form – largely discredited but it is generally taken for granted by science fiction writers.
That’s what I thought too, and then I stumbled onto this, later published in the American Economic Review. Fascinating.
Meredith 09.25.14 at 6:46 am
David in NY, do you mean that Kitty G’s murder didn’t happen in a city because it happened in Queens (and not, e.g., in Manhattan) or because it happened in a neighborhood (which is, of course, how city-dwellers experience their lives, whether living in Queens or Manhattan)?
J Thomas 09.25.14 at 10:53 am
The idea that there are two forms of Sapir-Whorf, strong and weak, and the strong form is false while the weak form is something to think about, is a language construct which is probably not particularly useful.
Whorf worked as an insurance adjuster, and he noticed accidents where people were careless with empty gasoline containers. The problem of course was that a full gasoline container can burn, while one that is full of gasoline vapor will explode.
Language is not a strait-jacket that forces people to think within its confines. The only people who are limited by the language they learn are people who are not willing to create language for themselves. The more that you create new language to fit your needs, the less limited you are.
harry b 09.25.14 at 12:40 pm
Reading Whorf’s wiki page is fascinating. What an interesting person. And his little brother directed the Beverley Hillbillies!
Mdc 09.25.14 at 1:11 pm
Meredith:
I forbid my students from using the term “the Greeks.”
bianca steele 09.25.14 at 2:20 pm
I found <a href="https://www.sfu.ca/cmns/courses/2012/801/1-Readings/Nicholson%20Shocking%20Masculinity%20Stanley%20Milgrim%20Obedience.pdf"this which is interesting especially because I was wondering last night how propensity to obey in the Milgram experiment might possibly correlate with the subject’s score on the Kohlberg “moral maturity” test, given the gender discrepancies in results. It’s possible to tell a story where men who score highly on the test are more moral–the official version–and so would ignore what the authority figure was saying to them. But if women, who generally score lower on the test, are more likely to ignore the authority figure, that would suggest Gilligan was right that the test wasn’t measuring what it was supposed to be, for women at least. Because it’s also possible that obeying authority is associated with the same traits that lead to a high score on the test. Obeying authority in an impersonal social structure is associated with maturity for men, in our society, after all, in something of the same way paying attention to the social group is associated with maturity for women.
I see there’s something here but the strong correlation between disobedience in the experiment, high scores on Kohlberg’s test, and participation in the 1964 Free Speech Movement might suggest something else is going on.
JanieM 09.25.14 at 2:21 pm
Meredith — I’ll be curious to see how David answers your question, but he didn’t say the murder didn’t happen in a city, he said the story didn’t happen in a city. It didn’t happen anywhere, because it wasn’t true.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 2:52 pm
Meredith — apologize for lack of specificity, but JanieM interprets correctly — the “story” — 38 people ignoring a woman in mortal distress — didn’t happen at all. But, as I keep trying to point out, there must be some reason that story won’t die, or is dying very slowly even among people we consider well-informed. And my hypothesis is that there’s a general, underlying assumption among Americans that cities are really bad places, where you are alone among strangers who would not come to your aid if you needed it.
LFC 09.25.14 at 3:02 pm
burritoboy @59
David @60
I suspect there’s variation: some people who live in suburbs may pretend they are small towns; others don’t. There are in fact certain places that have an ambiguous or dual status: i.e., they are incorporated towns w/ their own mayor and gov’t (and, in some perhaps rare-ish cases, sense of community) but are also, in effect, suburbs in/of a larger metro area.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 3:13 pm
And if I’m right that these zombie stories keep coming back because they are in line with unstated assumptions about society generally, I confess I’m not sure about the Milgrim situation. While Milgrim’s work may have been discredited in some ways — primarily ethically, but maybe others — I’m not sure that it has a “story” like the Genovese story that has been discredited. The take-away from Milgrim that I got was the view that large numbers of well-meaning people are unwilling to question authority, even when authority tells us to do something obviously harmful to others. That is, we are all (or most of us) potentially concentration camp guards.
This story (my own derived from the popular press of several decades ago) is an extrapolation from the actual Milgrim experiments. The actual experiments (I gather, having not done significant reading about them) may better be read as raising more subtle questions about the kind of persuasion, short of military or totalitarian discipline, that are sufficient to convince people to commit acts that are obviously harmful to others. I guess my conclusion is that it’s not clear that Milgrim’s conclusions were wrong in the first place (as the Genovese story was) or that a discredited account of his work is being perpetuated in college classes, for example. Thus, unlike the Genovese case, I’m not sure that the Milgrim experiments really raise the question that Harry was interested in — why doesn’t false stuff get corrected faster?
LFC 09.25.14 at 3:19 pm
burritoboy @59
Keep your eyes peeled and observe how American suburbs are designed.
I suspect there’s also a fair amt of variation here too, though am not familiar w the lit. on it. My guess wd be the more recent the suburb, the more it’s been designed to look like someone’s image of an archetypal town (w a certain amt of green space, a defined ‘downtown’ etc), though it’s a guess. [I’ve worked, albeit some yrs ago, in an actual small town in a chronically depressed (in the economic sense) part of the country, so I don’t have an esp. romantic image of small towns.]
David in NY 09.25.14 at 3:32 pm
LFC @ 74. Please. My view doesn’t depend on where people really live, in farms or in towns, suburbs, cities, whatever . It is, as I tried to explain, that there is a deep current of anti-urbanism in American society (certainly more present among those in rural areas or suburbs, but even present among urbanites — you should hear some New Yorkers complain about the city) , and that this underlying assumption facilitates the perpetuation of the Genovese myth.
I regret the “my little town” metaphor, intended ironically, and drawn from Paul Simon’s My Little Town:
I did wonder about one other possible reason for the perpuation of the Genovese myth, its potential appeal to the left, seeing the callousness of the 38 people as a symptom of the decay of bourgeois society. But the only evidence I have for this is a Phil Ochs song about the Genovese case, Outside of a Small Circle of Friends. See lyrics http://www.metrolyrics.com/outside-of-a-small-circle-of-friends-lyrics-phil-ochs.html. And although I wish Ochs’s views were powerful enough to still be affecting us, I really doubt it.
Peter Dorman 09.25.14 at 3:36 pm
Regarding the motivation for the longevity of the Genovese myth, has anyone else noticed there is a tendency for two strong factions to form around morally contentious political or social issues? One is “we are all guilty, the enemy is us”, the other “we are all virtuous, and anyone who denies this is an enemy”. One sees this among whites on this topic of racism, social divisions over climate change, war and imperialism, etc. I wonder whether there are deep-seated psychological forces at work. (I recognize that many, probably most, people align with neither faction. I sure don’t.)
The Genovese parable falls into the “we are all guilty” camp. It supposedly shows that virtually all of us will fail to step up to our ethical responsibilities at a moment of extreme need. You could also argue that the misinterpretation of Milgrom serves the same psychological purpose: we are all potential nazis, willing torturers in thrall to authority.
The “we are virtuous” side finds its expression in group identity, patriotism and sectarianism in particular, where it has a lot of political impact. It doesn’t show up so much in the social sciences any more, does it? There used to be celebratory myths about the virtues of ourselves and our ancestors, but these have fallen out of favor, at least on an academic or pseudo-academic level. Or am I missing something?
bianca steele 09.25.14 at 3:36 pm
@77
I’ve no doubt the Genovese story appeals to a wide variety of people, though the New Yorker article linked above, with its focus on Rosenthal’s motives for promoting it, suggests on the contrary the neoconservative “mugged by reality” narrative, along the lines of John Podhoretz’s rants about the Upper West Side of his childhood years. (Until I read that article, I knew nothing about the event other than the bare facts you quoted–“38 people heard and saw the attack, which happened on the sidewalk in full view of everyone and lasted half an hour or more
(all those facts false), and the woman’s name.)
Niall McAuley 09.25.14 at 3:38 pm
In college, I knew folks studying Philosophy and English, and I was laughing at them for studying Freud. But in fact they had to in order to properly appreciate all the philosophy and literature which takes Freud seriously.
It’s as if they had to study Schiaparelli and Lowell so that they could appreciate the Barsoom books, only less fun.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 4:26 pm
Peter Dorman @ 78. I’m not sure I buy your dichotomy, except that at least it would explain both Genovese and Milgrim situations. I guess I don’t really think many people are attracted to a “we’re all guilty” view, though. For example, it seemed to me that most people thought it really distressing that Milgrim had showed that so many of us might be guilty — that the Nazis might not have been just a historical anomaly. And in Genovese, it wasn’t all of us, but those city folks. But I’ve got nothing empirical on this.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 4:39 pm
Just to give some empirical support for my view about the deep current of anti-urbanism in American society:
http://nextcity.org/daily/entry/anti-city-quotes-americans-cities
Peter Dorman 09.25.14 at 5:12 pm
David, you could be right — I’m just speculating. My experience with both stories is that they are framed as saying something unfortunate about “human nature”, or perhaps the dismal state to which we’ve fallen here in the USA. Perhaps the stories are given meaning by different people for different reasons.
I’m attuned to the “we are all guilty” meme from working on the environmental front, where it has quite a bit of salience in some quarters.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 5:15 pm
Sure, Peter. Interesting about the environmental connection.
TM 09.25.14 at 5:46 pm
The disdain for, and fear of the city has been a constant of American culture at least since Jefferson. It could be though that it already “started with the Greeks” ;-)
But as somebody noted, the Genovese myth was started by the urban NYT.
burritoboy 09.25.14 at 7:03 pm
LFC,
David in NY has been posting up a lot of things that he and I are very much in agreement with. Specifically on your question, though there’s some variation over the years, most American suburbs are designed to be reminiscent of what is viewed as a small town or rural area. Now, what is viewed as a small town or rural area changes somewhat over time. Right now, having a mock main street is more in fashion. Nevertheless, the ideology around the design of the American suburb is fairly stable – it’s a rejection of the city and attempts to recreate small town or rural life. There’s a lot of cross-pollination with England on this, and many early American suburbs are copies of, or variations on, the earliest English suburbs of the late 19th and early 20th centuries. It’s not unusual for the early American suburbs to be very explicit and nostalgic copies, down to street names being ersatz English ones, Tudor style architecture, mock-Gothic chapels, the schools being (in both architecture and curriculum) heavily influenced by English public school models, golfing and other English sports, etc.
burritoboy 09.25.14 at 7:17 pm
Bianca Steele (@79),
Yes, Podhoretz’ “My Negro Problem – and Ours” was published the year before the Kitty Genovese murder. However, both these things can be viewed within a lens of the huge debate of the time concerning the American city – James Q. Wilson’s 1963 The American City, for just one other example out of many possible ones.
Note: “My Negro Problem – and Ours” is about Podhoretz’ childhood in Bed-Stuy, not the UWS.
bianca steele 09.25.14 at 7:47 pm
burritoboy:
“My Negro Problem – And Ours” was written by Norman Podhoretz. His son, John, is now also an editor at Commentary and has written about how people on the Upper West Side, like his family, felt about the not yet gentrified area.
It occurs to me that possibly the best narrative for the Genovese story might be “the safest place for a young unmarried girl to live by herself is a ‘traditional’ Italian neighborhood with lots of violence but also chivalry,” but only an insider would know from the neighborhood name itself that Kitty Genovese was not living in that kind of place.
David in NY 09.25.14 at 8:17 pm
burritoboy —
I didn’t know that Wilson wrote “The American City” and only read some of his later stuff on crime. Is it good? I’ll have to look at it in my upcoming leisure time. Am I right in remembering that he formulated something like the “Broken Windows” theory that others have mentioned as another in the discredited, zombie ideas, or am I wrong in connecting him to it?
David in NY 09.25.14 at 8:30 pm
Never mind. Wilson wrote a piece “Broken Windows,” along with George Zelling, in the Atlantic, 3/1/82. I don’t know if that’s the earliest formulation of the notion (I thought I’d heard of it earlier), but he surely gave a name to it.
LFC 09.25.14 at 8:30 pm
burritoboy @85
Specifically on your question, though there’s some variation over the years, most American suburbs are designed to be reminiscent of what is viewed as a small town or rural area.
It’s possible we are using or defining “suburb” differently, but I’ve spent substantial chunks (though not all) of my life living in (what I would call) suburbs that have little resemblance to what you are describing. These are places that date from the immediate post-WW2 pd; how typical or atypical they are I’m not sure, but they do not esp. strike me as attempts to “reject the city and recreate small-town or rural life,” and I can assure you that not all American suburbs have Tudor-style architecture, mock-Gothic chapels (!), schools modeled on English public schools, etc. This will prob. be my last comment on this, since it’s OT the main topic of the thread, insofar as there is one. (Plus I really don’t care about the subject that much, tbh.)
LFC 09.25.14 at 8:41 pm
David @77 (just saw this):
My view doesn’t depend on where people really live, in farms or in towns, suburbs, cities, whatever . It is, as I tried to explain, that there is a deep current of anti-urbanism in American society (certainly more present among those in rural areas or suburbs, but even present among urbanites — you should hear some New Yorkers complain about the city) , and that this underlying assumption facilitates the perpetuation of the Genovese myth.
That may be entirely right. (Your initial statement made me think that where people actually lived was or might be relevant, but in this formulation it isn’t.)
David in NY 09.25.14 at 8:55 pm
LFC @ 91. Oh, thanks for noting that. I was beginning to feel that my ability to communicate was failing seriously.
TM 09.25.14 at 9:27 pm
90: There isn’t really a consistent definition of “suburb”. I have spent way too much time reading the urbanism literature and found that there are a bunch of different definitions that are conflicting with each other and are rarely applied consistently even within a paper. Briefly, any definition based on city boundaries is arbitrary because some cities have annexed their suburbs and other haven’t. The Census definitions of urban area and metropolitan area are not consistent over time and also aren’t always helpful. For example, a Census Metropolitan Area can contain whole counties that are classified as rural. Some attempts at classifying suburbs lead to functioning, independent cities classified as suburbs simply because of their size. (Of course there tend to be interdependencies between geographically close cities but that shouldn’t be enough to relegate all but the biggest city to suburb status.) I wrote a technical paper about these distinctions: http://www.slideshare.net/amenning/geospatial-analysis-of-us-census-data
Phil 09.25.14 at 11:41 pm
David @90: yes, “Broken Windows” by Wilson & Kelling was where it all started. Have a look at it – it’s findable on the Net – and try and follow the argument through; it goes to some weird places.
About the “we are all potential camp guards” message from the Milgram study (I forget now whether Milgram drew anything like this inference or if it attached itself to his results later on). It’s really very odd. The process Milgram put his volunteers through certainly rings some very unpleasant bells – it’s analogous to some sort of nightmarish sadistic seduction or grooming process (“Now slap him. Good! Now slap him again. Harder this time. No, don’t stop now. You must continue”…). But did people imagine that this was how camp guards were recruited, or trained? It wouldn’t work – it wouldn’t scale, apart from anything else – unless the SS were some kind of warrior cult or S/M Theban Band. Which, perhaps fortunately for the world, they weren’t.
In reality, as far as I can tell, people were induced to commit atrocities through a combination of ideological commitment, esprit de corps, direct orders and constant background threat. If you wanted to step aside from firing squad duty, you could, this time, but next time you might be less fortunate; next time you might be a lot less fortunate.
Meredith 09.26.14 at 5:09 am
Coming back so late. Sorry. David in NY, I think you are onto something. I would suggest adding the U.S. east-west drift of notions of rural. So that colonial English notions of commons, fences, and so forth get transferred westward but also modified (you don’t even try to fence the prairie you’ve just plowed for the first time, for instance — besides, you’re growing wheat, not penning in swine, at least at first). Subsequent immigrants are “accepted,” eventually, on the basis of how rural they are. Nordic people in Minnesota or South Dakota in the late 1800’s may have been looked down on by the Anglos already there, but soon they would be celebrated for having their feet and hands in the soil. Contrast how immigrants who worked in cities, as the industrial revolution got under way, were viewed by Anglos in those cities and by all sorts of people in still very rural areas west (and south).
This longing for “the country”, for agriculture, and its regional associations (e.g., “East” = city) needs lots more examination. Especially since agriculture is so f-ing hard. Really, what’s so wonderful about it? (Ask I, the gardener!)
My Oregon-born-and-raised mother-in-law, who raised her own children in Oklahoma and Texas, ventured east of the Mississippi for the first time when our son (her first grandchild) was born in 1980. She looked on in wonder at (today’s version of) rural New England. “It’s so green! Like (coastal) Oregon!” She’d expected nothing “back East” but concrete.
Two of my husband’s siblings live in Colorado, where the corridor between Denver and Fort Collins is more densely inhabited than the corridor between NYC and Boston. But don’t even try to persuade them of that fact. By definition, still, east is concrete, west is rural.
Another of my husband’s siblings has lived in Alaska for nearly 40 years. Same story, more so. I don’t know how to talk to all these folks about my Minnesota grandfather, grandson of a man born in Providence, RI (his own grandfather a Revolutionary war veteran) who worked in Alaska in the 1930’s. Though I do know this: we all grow gardens. (Even half of my children in NYC cultivate what they can there, even if only pots on the fire escape.)
Meredith 09.26.14 at 5:24 am
Don’t mean to get weirdly OT. I think this is truly on-topic (of comments, if not of OP):
http://www.newrepublic.com/article/119571/gabby-giffords-gun-control-ads-are-criticized-because-theyre-working
David in NY 09.26.14 at 1:44 pm
Phil @ 95
I have always just assumed that the splash that Milgram’s work made, in the popular mind at least, was due to a fairly direct (if incorrect) analogy to the Nazi concentration and extermination camps. But for that (and the ethical problems it raised), I think, his stuff might well have been ignored. I gather that, as you point out, his work is not necessarily relevant to such situations. Indeed, it’s a little unclear to me to what, in real life, it is really relevant (beyond maybe a cult as you propose).
Similarly, I think, Wilson and Kelling’s article, which I’ve read cursorily, was later characterized by Wilson as “speculation” (1996 article). He rejected the “broken windows” theory as applied — it became a “zero tolerance” policing technique resulting in greatly increased misdemeanor arrests — and proposed a much different policing regime, involving a real connection of the police to the community, rather than the adversary relationship that the “zero tolerance” attitude inevitably produced. I think Wilson would really have been unhappy that the lead for all his obituaries was that he was the founder of the “broken windows” theory of policing, which had become a repressive practice of which he did not really approve.
Both cases are instances of scholarly, or near-scholarly, work that appealed to the popular mind at a particular moment — post-WWII US, post-’60’s crime increase — when they seemed to say more than they really did about those situations.
TM 09.26.14 at 2:06 pm
“This longing for “the countryâ€, for agriculture, and its regional associations (e.g., “East†= city) needs lots more examination. Especially since agriculture is so f-ing hard. Really, what’s so wonderful about it?”
Hint: Jefferson didn’t do a lot of farming himself.
gatherdust 09.26.14 at 5:27 pm
It’s interesting that Arendt and Milgram have suffered similarly for different reasons or is it differently for similar reasons? Arendt. Arendt’s legacy lies in underscoring that that there’s an ordinariness even among the most diabolical. But this upsets our equation that great evil must be done by great evildoers. Milgram’s legacy is that among us ordinary mortals lies the situational ability to do extraordinary evil. His critics are upset at his attempt to undue the equation of ordinary good people are only capable of doing ordinary good things. There’s no definitive social psychological research basis for this either but only some things are subject to the need for evidence.
I don’t know what philosophers do (or are doing) with Milgram but if its philosophy then it’s largely unrelated to social psychological, theory, research, or practice. And I gather, as some have already mentioned, problems in Milgram’s research practices don’t discredit his work insofar as it has a record of replicated results.
Milgram was a Yale and thus the research was initially done in New Haven. He took the show on the road to Bridgeport. Not Harvard, Boston, or New York. Talk about memory that adheres to default biases.
gatherdust 09.26.14 at 5:31 pm
Wilson was not particularly impressed with criminological theory. Except for the notions of social disorganization that emanated from the 1920s and 1930s. And his ideas read like an update of social disorganzation The key difference is that sociologists were a bit more upbeat about the possibilities of ameliorating the mess. Wilson represented the frustrated who sought to return to an earlier criminology where good and bad were clearer.
gatherdust 09.26.14 at 5:42 pm
D in Ny @98 Milgram’s stature is more than an Esquire article and the reawakening of what would become a cultural fascination with the Holocaust. Then again, my attitude about James Q. Wilson contains about the same amount enthusiasm as you have for Milgram. Wilson came late to a realization that social disorganization assumes the need for great repression. To bad for all the lives wasted to satisfy the fears and biases of a law and order white public and a willing bunch of criminologists who never wavered from their understanding that crime is something only criminals do.
Bruce Webb 09.28.14 at 7:09 am
“In linguistics, the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis is – at least in its strong form – largely discredited but it is generally taken for granted by science fiction writers.”
Well it is worth noting that Whorf never endorsed the “strong form” of Sapir-Whorf nor on my reading did Sapir. Instead it was the result of a deliberate misreading of Whorf that amounted to a strawman.
Similarly the dismissal of Whorf as “an insurance adjuster” is part and parcel of the same strawman narrative. Instead Whorf was called in to do investigations that today would be labled as those of an “engineer” or an “forensic investigator”.
Whorf’s real crime was not to be some amateur pronouncing a theory of unintelligibility between languages. Because he wasn’t an amateur in any but the 19th century sense and never suggested that learners of say Hopi could never master the thought processes of English or vice versa (indeed Whorf in his papers attempted a “translation” that ‘Strong’ Sapir-Whorf would hold impossible). Instead Whorf’s work cast grave doubt on the fundamentals of Logical Positivism writ large and the belief almost universally held by both the practioners of the new field of Linguistics and the newish strains of philosophy of the Analytical Philosophers that you could approach capital R reality through the rigid application of language mediated by techniques of formal logic. Think Russell and the Early Wittgenstein and the Cambridge School. Or the supposedly contrasting work of the Ordinary Language Philosophers led by Austin at Oxford. If Whorf was correct then you would not be able to explicate reality just by listening to upper class Englishmen speaking their language out loud to each other in the Fellows’ Garden at some Oxford College. Nor would you be able to embrace later theories like Chomskite Generative Linguistics that assumed and asserted that there was a universal hard wired set of language rules shared by all human beings if you could only get deep enough in the rules that ultimately generated the superficially different languages actually extant.
The whole line of theory that infused academic Linguistics and much of Anglo-American analytical philosophy in the first half of the 20th century would have been undermined by taking Whorf’s work seriously. So it wasn’t.
Making Benjamin Whorf an honorary member of DeLongs Ancient and Hermetic Order of the Shrill. Or maybe per Digby just another DFH. Someone not to be taken seriously by the ‘Sensible’ and the Mandarins. Something made easier by not actually reading anything he actually wrote.
Johnny Pez 09.28.14 at 5:50 pm
As far as I know, Harlan Ellison has spent the last fifty years being pissed off about the Genovese story. It’s not because he’s anti-urban; it’s just that, being Harlan Ellison, he needed something to be royally pissed off about, and the Genovese murder fit the bill. I don’t know if anyone ever told him the story was a lie.
RJ 09.29.14 at 1:25 pm
Mr. Webb, are you sure that there is or ever was anyone who believes that you can “explicate reality just by listening to upper class Englishmen speaking their language out loud to each other in the Fellows’ Garden at some Oxford College”. I’ve never heard anyone claim this thesis. Certainly, as a scholar of recent working-class origin, it certainly is not why I am impressed by early analytical philosophy.
A lot of people at Crooked Timber seem very eager to cast analytical philosophy as the sole purview of minor nobles with nothing to do. For those of us coming from a practical, scientific background, analytical philosophy actually is quite resourceful, not just as theory, but on the ground. I have not read Whorf’s own work, and while it may place certain aspects of analytical philosophy and its offshoots into question, it is unlikely that would undermine all of the great resources accruing from analytical philosophy.
bianca steele 09.29.14 at 11:32 pm
I drafted and deleted a couple tries at a comment about how difficult it is to tell whether anything bad is happening when people are walking back to campus late at night, say, from an off-campus party. Sadly, since then the campus near my house has been in the news for a sexual assault. I suppose it could have happened at the party at a house 3/4 mi away that I could hear all day Saturday. Or anywhere else, really.
Since the OP went up, I began to suspect “the official account” is straight out of Malcolm Gladwell, and as it turns out, it is, an airtight version that allows little room for objections (though Gladwell finds one anyway).
Comments on this entry are closed.